A Plague of Prisons: The Epidemiology of Mass Incarceration in America

A Plague of Prisons: The Epidemiology of Mass Incarceration in America

by Ernest Drucker
A Plague of Prisons: The Epidemiology of Mass Incarceration in America

A Plague of Prisons: The Epidemiology of Mass Incarceration in America

by Ernest Drucker

eBook

$14.99  $19.99 Save 25% Current price is $14.99, Original price is $19.99. You Save 25%.

Available on Compatible NOOK devices, the free NOOK App and in My Digital Library.
WANT A NOOK?  Explore Now

Related collections and offers

LEND ME® See Details

Overview

The public health expert and prison reform activist offers “meticulous analysis” on our criminal justice system and the plague of American incarceration (The Washington Post).
 
An internationally recognized public health scholar, Ernest Drucker uses the tools of epidemiology to demonstrate that incarceration in the United States has become an epidemic—a plague upon our body politic. He argues that imprisonment, originally conceived as a response to the crimes of individuals, has become “mass incarceration”: a destabilizing force that damages the very social structures that prevent crime.
 
Drucker tracks the phenomenon of mass incarceration using basic public health concepts—“incidence and prevalence,” “outbreaks,” “contagion,” “transmission,” “potential years of life lost.” The resulting analysis demonstrates that our unprecedented rates of incarceration have the contagious and self-perpetuating features of the plagues of previous centuries.
 
Sure to provoke debate and shift the paradigm of how we think about punishment, A Plague of Prisons offers a novel perspective on criminal justice in twenty-first-century America.
 
“How did America’s addiction to prisons and mass incarceration get its start and how did it spread from state to state? Of the many attempts to answer this question, none make as much sense as the explanation found in [this] book.” —The Philadelphia Inquirer

Product Details

ISBN-13: 9781595589538
Publisher: New Press, The
Publication date: 07/19/2019
Sold by: Barnes & Noble
Format: eBook
Pages: 242
File size: 5 MB

About the Author

Ernest Drucker is a scholar in residence and senior research associate at John Jay College of Criminal Justice, City University of New York. He is professor emeritus of family and social medicine at Montefiore Medical Center/Albert Einstein College of Medicine and adjunct professor of epidemiology at Columbia University's Mailman School of Public Health. He is an NIH-funded researcher, editor-in-chief of the international Harm Reduction Journal, a Fulbright Senior Specialist in Global Health, and a Soros Justice Fellow. He is also a founder and former chairman of Doctors of the World/USA. He lives in New York City.

Read an Excerpt

CHAPTER 1

AN EPIDEMIOLOGICAL RIDDLE

An "unusual event" has occurred in which a great loss of life has taken place. The population involved in the event was large and very diverse: men and women, adults and children, different social classes — the rich, the middle class, and the poor. Can we use the available data — a few details about who lived and who died — along with some tools of epidemiology (the science of public health) to figure out what that "unusual event" was? Here are the things we know:

• Over two-thirds of the more than two thousand people involved died.

• Among the adult population, women were three times as likely to survive as men.

• The children under twelve years of age were almost 50 percent more likely to survive than the adults.

• Those in the highest social class were 50 percent more likely to survive than the middle class, and over twice as likely to survive as the lower class.

What was the event? A lethal new virus? An act of terrorism or war? A natural disaster? An accident? How can this sparse "mortality data" on the differences between those who survived and those who perished point the way to the solution?

Epidemiologists use tables to organize data systematically in a way that reflects details about all the individuals exposed to an event (or disease), sorted out by who died and who survived. The two outcomes, life and death, can be categorized and cross-referenced by gender, age, and economic status to give a portrait of how each one affects an individual's odds of death or survival, and how these three variables interact with each other.

Figure 1.1 gives us the basic mortality data from this event expressed as rates. Of the 2,224 people involved in our unusual event, 1,513, or 68 percent, died, and 711, or 32 percent (fewer than one in three), survived. The actual number of deaths can be deceptive — it's the rates that matter: the proportion who survived and its inverse, the proportion who died.

What do we know from this first piece of evidence that can help us solve the puzzle? First off, 68 percent — what epidemiologists call the overall or crude mortality rate — is a very high proportion of deaths for any disease or disaster. For an idea of what this death rate signifies, recall that over 2,800 people died in the World Trade Center attack of 9/11, but about 10,000 were in the buildings when the planes struck. The mortality rate of any event that takes many lives is the number of deaths divided by the number of people exposed — the total number at risk for death. So we can say the entire population in the WTC buildings was at risk at the time the planes struck, and that the 28 percent mortality rate in the WTC attack is the proportion that died among all those who were exposed — less than half the mortality rate of our mystery event.

Was our unusual event a particular outbreak of an epidemic disease? Few long-known diseases kill such a large proportion of those who get infected (e.g., malaria eventually kills about 25 percent, but over many years). But some newer diseases (such as Ebola) kill a much higher proportion; of those who are infected with the Ebola virus, about 90 percent quickly die. So our event could be an outbreak of a new, very lethal virus that struck a village of two thousand people. Or maybe it is some sort of accident — a train or plane crash? Many plane crashes have a 100 percent mortality rate, but in some cases all survive (Sullenberger's remarkable landing in the Hudson River in the winter of 2009, for example). In most plane crashes, many die but, on average, two-thirds survive. However, no plane holds two thousand people, so that's an unlikely answer. But some crowded commuter trains hold more than that. Could it be a huge accident? A terrorist bombing? It could be a wartime battle, where tens of thousands can die, or the tsunami of 2008 in Indonesia, or the Haitian earthquake of 2010 that killed over three hundred thousand — huge numbers with very high death rates similar to those of our event.

But how do we account for the differences in death rate by age, gender, and social class? What disease or disaster would produce this particular pattern of death rates? One of the most common factors affecting health, life expectancy, and the risk for many diseases is gender — only women die in childbirth or get cervical cancer, but many more men get lung cancer, and most casualties of combat are still males.

In the case of our mystery event, both men and women were involved (i.e., exposed to risk), and there were many more males than females in the at-risk population — 1,690 adult males (76 percent of the total) vs. 425 adult females (20 percent). Looking at the mortality data in Figure 1.1 as rates, we can see immediately that gender made a big difference in one's chances of survival. Females were almost three times as likely to survive as males: 80 percent of the men died vs. 26 percent of the females.

What could account for that? A disease that affects both sexes but is much more lethal for men? For specific diseases, the death rates are called case fatality rates (CFRs), because the only people at risk for dying are those already diagnosed with the disease — that is, the cases. What diseases that affect both men and women would have such different CFRs for the two sexes? Maybe the sample involved in this particular event could have been exposed to the risk differently based on some differences associated with their gender — for example, a workplace where most of the men were involved in something dangerous, say, a toxic product that caused cancer. Or an accident where men and women were segregated in some way that caused the men to bear the brunt of whatever was responsible — an explosion in some part of the building they all worked in, but that housed more of the men. All but thirty of the 146 victims of the Triangle Shirtwaist fire of 1911 were young women workers, while the two factory owners — Max Blanck and Isaac Harris — fled to the roof and survived. Is that a model for our event?

Looking at mortality rates by age offers more clues. In diseases that generally affect both children and adults, usually it's the very young and the oldest who have the highest death rates, as is the case with influenza. But that virus acts very differently depending on the strain: the disastrous 1918 Spanish flu epidemic that killed over 50 million had the highest CFR among healthy, young adult men. In our event, all that we are given is the fact that there were 109 children under twelve and 2,115 adults — so only 5 percent of the exposed population were children. But somehow they had the best chance of survival: while only 31 percent of all the adults survived, 52 percent of the children lived (a 45 percent difference in CFR). What could account for this?

Most deadly events like hurricanes and infectious diseases also strike the very old and the very young hardest — people who are the frailest and least able to fight for their own survival. Again, it could be something about the children and the women that exposed them both differentially to whatever is killing all these people — maybe some of the children were with the women who survived, being helped by them. We still need more data — especially something that would tell us more about what common feature differentiates among these 2,224 individuals with such a dangerous exposure to a potentially highly lethal risk.

The population exposed in our event was a very mixed group. Their social and economic status is given to us as upper class, 325; middle class, 285; and lower class, 706. Another group, the largest, was not classified by social class at all, but we have information that suggests they were all from either the middle or lower class. Figure 1.2 shows that for these different social groups there seems to be a clear trend: being richer increased your chances of survival dramatically. Sixty-two percent of the upper class survived, versus 41 percent of the middle class, 25 percent of the lower class, and 23 percent of the others (suggesting their risk was similar to that of the lower class).

One of the most important predictors of any population's health is social class, which generally correlates to higher levels of income, education, and housing — all factors that confer protection against disease. We know that social class affects health and life expectancy — in the United States the poorest third of the population has a life expectancy about ten years lower than the richest third.

If we combine the risks faced by each individual, there is actually much more information to be gleaned from the data we have. Examining these factors together in a single table (as in Figure 1.2) allows us to combine several of our variables and show the risk associated with different combinations of factors. After all, that's who we really are — we each have a gender and an age and a social class. If each operates separately to affect life and death, we may now ask, how do they work in combination in this event?

From the tables shown thus far we can see that the factors that predict better chances of survival — gender, age, and social class — seem to act together in a most dramatic way. In every social class, women were far more likely to survive than men, but upper- and middle-class women were twice as likely to survive as poorer women. For children, the effect of social class on survival was even more pronounced — all of the upper- and middle-class children survived, as compared to only one-third of the poorer children. We can now see that social class was a lifesaver. Independent of age or gender, class affected every group's odds of survival. So maybe that is the key — or at least a link — to explaining these huge differences in chance of survival.

Hint: Location, Location, Location

While social class alone may not account for different survival rates, perhaps social class in this event serves as a proxy for something else that explains survival. Could social position reflect actual location? Sometimes the difference between life and death involves chance or circumstance — where you are seated in a car or a plane that crashes, for instance. Sometimes it's about decisions made by people on the spot — for example, when triage is employed on the battlefield. In the World Trade Center attack, where you were located in the buildings made a huge difference in odds for survival: for those on or above the floors that the planes struck — floors 94 to 98 in the North Tower and 78 to 84 in the South Tower — over 95 percent died.

While epidemic diseases and catastrophes may take many lives, they never act in a vacuum — the social characteristics of the population have a large role in determining risk or exposure to harm. In the World Trade Center the offices on the upper floors were more expensive to rent and had the most prosperous tenants. So what could be the circumstances of our mystery event that catalyzed the effects of social class so powerfully and still conferred substantial additional protection on women and children?

By now you may be able to guess what the unusual event was that took so many lives so unequally. On April 10, 1912, the Titanic left Southampton, England, on her maiden voyage to New York City. As befit the ship's name, Titanic was the largest ship afloat, the most luxurious ship of the day. The White Star Line called it "the safest ship ever built" — so safe that she carried only twenty lifeboats, with space for less than half of her complement of more than two thousand passengers and crew. The ship's construction in the Belfast yards was believed to have made her unsinkable, so her lifeboats were deemed a bit of noblesse oblige — necessary only to rescue the survivors of other, less invincible ships that could actually sink. Besides, lifeboats took up valuable deck and cabin space that could be sold to passengers.

By now everyone knows that four days into the voyage, at 11:40 P.M. on the night of April 14, the Titanic struck an iceberg, tearing a hundred-yard breach below the waterline. Soon the icy water of the North Atlantic began to pour into the ship. It became obvious that many would not be able to get into a lifeboat, and the life jackets issued on deck would not offer any protection at all when passengers were exposed to water four degrees below freezing. The "unsinkable" Titanic slid beneath the waters two hours and forty minutes after hitting the iceberg.

Subsequent inquiries attributed the high loss of life to "an insufficient number of lifeboats and inadequate training in their use." As the epidemiological patterns of life and death reveal, we know now that there was much more to it. The circumstances leading up to the deadly outcome have been described over and over in print and most recently as a hugely successful film, with the event characterized variously as a tragedy, a catastrophe, and a preventable man-made disaster. Now a whole new generation knows the story of the Titanic, but as the romance of two star-crossed young lovers. Like Romeo and Juliet, these two play out the larger tale of social class and conflicts — with all the huge social disparities visible in the passengers and their place in the Victorian social hierarchy of the day — expressed now in where they were located in the ship when it hit ice.

On the Titanic's maiden voyage, the passenger list included a mixture of the world's wealthiest, basking in the spacious and elegant first-class accommodations, while hundreds of poor immigrants bound for America were packed far below deck into steerage at fares a fraction of those in first class. The crew were placed in quarters even less commodious and even deeper in the bowels of the ship — farther from the escape routes in the event of disaster.

While there was adequate time for most to get to the lifeboats, there was room in them for only one-third of the ship's population, so two-thirds died. The shortage of lifeboats was indeed the primary reason why so many perished, but the lack of lifeboats is only the tip of another iceberg made of darker stuff: the brutally rigid social structure of the age — the huge and virtually inviolable disparities between rich and poor in Victorian Britain — and how that was reflected in the location and crowding of the various levels of accommodations on the Titanic. At the same time, the better survival rates for women and children, independent of social class, reflect the ethical customs of the time: women and children first. Like a social X-ray or MRI, epidemiology is able to look past the outer skin of a large-scale event to see its inner structure of cultural values, which are always important determinants of the outcomes of any epidemic or disaster — often more powerful in deciding who lives and who dies than the material circumstance surrounding the event.

The Titanic disaster is a perfect illustration of the power of epidemiology to reveal the inner workings behind events that take many lives. Organizing information this way exposes the personal, social, and environmental conditions that are the backdrop to all epidemics and to most large-scale disasters — especially of the man-made variety. These conditions often explain what happened to whom (and why) far more meaningfully than biological factors alone. Surely some characteristics of individual victims may seal their fates, but the specific context and history of large-scale disasters makes individual vulnerabilities and risks into epidemiology. Think of the faulty levees in Hurricane Katrina, or the system of sweatshop conditions responsible for the Triangle fire. The victims of these disastrous events were not randomly chosen for their fate.

(Continues…)


Excerpted from "A Plague of Prisons"
by .
Copyright © 2013 Ernest Drucker.
Excerpted by permission of The New Press.
All rights reserved. No part of this excerpt may be reproduced or reprinted without permission in writing from the publisher.
Excerpts are provided by Dial-A-Book Inc. solely for the personal use of visitors to this web site.

Table of Contents

Acknowledgments,
1. An Epidemiological Riddle,
2. Cholera in London: The Ghost Maps of Dr. Snow,
3. AIDS: The Epidemiology of a New Disease,
4. A Different Kind of Epidemic,
5. Anatomy of an Outbreak: New York's Rockefeller Drug Laws and the Prison Pump,
6. Orders of Magnitude: The Relative Impact of Mass Incarceration,
7. A Self-Sustaining Epidemic: Modes of Reproduction,
8. Chronic Incapacitation: The Long Tail of Mass Incarceration,
9. The Contagion of Punishment: Collateral Damage to Children and Families of Prisoners,
10. Ending Mass Incarceration: A Public Health Model,
Afterword to the Paperback Edition: The Future of Mass Incarceration in America,
Notes,
Index,

From the B&N Reads Blog

Customer Reviews